体检应该挂什么科| 晚上九点是什么时辰| 慰问金是什么意思| 猫猴子是什么| 下巴底下长痘痘是什么原因| 煜什么意思| 乳腺增生1类什么意思| 感冒了可以吃什么水果| 佳什么意思| 胃穿孔是什么原因引起的| 角化型脚气用什么药| 正常人吃叶酸有什么好处| 妒忌什么意思| 怀孕什么不能吃| 晚上9点到11点是什么时辰| 天空像什么| 隐血是什么意思| 疟疾病的症状是什么样| 什么是电解质饮料| 消费税是什么| 诺如病毒吃什么药最有效| 做梦梦到水是什么征兆| 捋一捋是什么意思| 什么东西不能一起吃| 泡脚有什么好处| 麻长什么样子图片| 眼眶发青是什么原因| 身体怕冷什么原因| 碳酸盐质玉是什么玉| 总感觉饿是什么原因| 吃什么可以控制血糖| 原来是什么意思| 开火上下结构念什么| 北京大学校长什么级别| 94岁属什么| 肚子胀什么原因| 1974年属什么生肖| 万艾可是什么药| 睡前吃香蕉有什么好处| 陈皮泡酒喝有什么功效和作用| 什么是跑马| 吃了头孢不能吃什么| 矬子是什么意思| 05年属鸡的是什么命| 有什么植物| 脾胃虚寒吃什么中成药| 小孩子经常流鼻血是什么原因| 蜡笔小新的爸爸叫什么| 胸闷出汗是什么原因| 艾灸能治什么| 如初是什么意思| 为什么恐龙会灭绝| 孕妇甲胎蛋白偏高说明什么| 峻字五行属什么| RH阳性什么意思| 三个女人一台戏什么意思| 可乐鸡翅需要什么材料| fob价格是什么意思| 4月29号是什么星座的| 豆绿色配什么颜色好看| 肠痈是什么病| 口腔溃疡吃什么好| 嘴唇发黑是什么症状| 头发没有光泽是什么原因| 舌头长泡吃什么药| 梦见老鼠是什么意思| 什么是代词| 梦到认识的人死了是什么意思| 心三联是指什么| 幼犬可以吃什么| 晚上血压高是什么原因| 入职体检前要注意什么| 8月15日什么星座| 背上长毛是什么原因引起的| 纳闷是什么意思| 封闭抗体是什么意思| 为什么要小心AB型血的人| 男人很man是什么意思| 射手座和什么座最配| 漆皮是什么皮| 葫芦是什么生肖| 臭虫最怕什么| pc什么意思| 什么是指标到校| 省委巡视组组长什么级别| 脑干诱发电位检查是检查什么| 咳嗽买什么药| 一什么露珠| 孕妇血压低吃什么能补上来| 白粉虱用什么药| 身上痒但是什么都没有| 耳浴10分钟什么意思| 早上四点是什么时辰| 背债是什么意思| 平顶山为什么叫平顶山| 肛门瘙痒用什么药| dazzle是什么牌子| 睡觉憋气是什么原因引起的| 繁衍的衍是什么意思| 尿道口痛什么原因| 布朗是什么水果| cvm是什么意思| 羊肚菌有什么功效和作用| 什么叫大男子主义| 公因数是什么意思| 盆腔积液是什么| 言字旁与什么有关| 梦见办丧事是什么兆头| 守是什么生肖| 冬至要注意什么| 吸烟有害健康为什么国家还生产烟| 吃什么能补肾| bishop是什么意思| 尽收眼底是什么意思| 尿急是什么症状| 耳堵耳闷是什么原因| 氯雷他定为什么比西替利嗪贵| 为什么减肥不掉秤| 决明子和什么搭配最好| 发际线高的人说明什么| 被蚂蚁咬了擦什么药| 椎间盘膨隆是什么意思| 女性外阴瘙痒用什么药| 山竹吃了有什么好处| 什么叫肠化| 白细胞高一点点是什么原因| 什么茶叶好| mlb是什么品牌| 彩色的什么| 外痔是什么样子的| 朝野是什么意思| 甲钴胺片主治什么病| 舌头裂纹吃什么药| 最聪明的狗是什么狗| 回南天是什么时候| 6个月宝宝可以吃什么辅食| 孤帆远影碧空尽的尽是什么意思| 身上瘙痒是什么原因| 下巴下面长痘痘是什么原因| 人为什么要工作| u1是什么意思| 下面长痘痘是什么原因| 正确的三观是什么| 什么叫非甾体抗炎药| 荨麻疹吃什么药效果好| 省略号的作用是什么| 安全期一般是什么时候| 于谦为什么加入国民党| 芦笋不能和什么一起吃| 刚生完宝宝的产妇吃什么好| 词牌名是什么意思| 均码是什么意思| 做梦梦见剪头发是什么意思| 为什么长痱子| bbq是什么| 眼睛充血用什么眼药水最好| 甲状腺4b级是什么意思| 小手指麻木是什么原因引起的| 手指甲有竖纹什么原因| 结核杆菌dna检测是检查什么| 地贫吃什么补血最快| 什么是心肌炎| 杭州菜属于什么菜系| 梦见别人穿红衣服是什么意思| 冠脉ct和冠脉造影有什么区别| 甘油三酯低是什么原因| 申时左眼跳是什么预兆| 脾虚湿热吃什么药| 物化是什么意思| 流清鼻涕吃什么药| 怀女孩有什么征兆| 脑壳疼是什么原因| 幼儿园中班学什么| 为什么嘴里发苦| 冶游史是什么意思| 什么是化学阉割| lad是什么意思| 春天能干什么| 什么是射频| 脚底有黑痣有什么说法| 血糖高什么水果可以吃| 痛风是什么病| 减脂晚上吃什么好| 肾痛吃什么药| 总是拉肚子是什么原因| 胰岛素高是什么原因| 胸部疼挂什么科| 吃什么不掉头发| 什么实实| 子宫内膜炎用什么药效果好| 一抹是什么意思| 愤是什么生肖| 为什么现在| 心衰为什么会引起水肿| 扁平疣用什么药膏管用| 抑菌是什么意思| 二月一号是什么星座| 为什么声音老是嘶哑| 冠状动脉ct检查什么| 上吐下泻吃什么好| 早上4点是什么时辰| 梦见买车是什么意思| 心脏传导阻滞吃什么药| 大水冲了龙王庙什么意思| 农历五月初五是什么节日| 1947年属什么| 啐了一口是什么意思| 什么叫手淫| 冰藤席是什么材质| 什么是对冲| 太阳花什么时候开花| 七月二十九是什么星座| 三月初什么星座| 去胎毒吃什么最好| 嘴唇干燥是什么原因引起的| 类风湿关节炎吃什么药效果好| 尿酸高要注意什么饮食| rr过低是什么意思| 7月4是什么星座| 睡觉为什么磨牙| 下肢浮肿是什么原因引起的| 榴莲不可以和什么食物一起吃| 1995年的猪五行属什么| 为什么打喷嚏会漏尿| 猪蹄子炖什么好吃| 九头身是什么意思| 护士节送什么花| 慢性盆腔炎吃什么药效果好| 为什么叫印度三哥| 宫禁糜烂用什么药| 冰晶是什么| 斑鱼是什么鱼| 否命题和命题的否定有什么区别| 梦见吃排骨是什么意思| 人怕冷是什么原因| 什么叫pc| 火龙果和香蕉榨汁有什么功效| 河南为什么简称豫| 欣喜若狂是什么意思| 金风送爽是什么意思| 男人喝红糖水有什么好处| 2020是什么年| 头臀径是指什么| 加油站为什么不能打电话| 吃什么精力旺盛有精神| 紫癜有什么危害| 助听器什么牌子好| 258什么意思| 什么药膏可以去黑头| 仕女图是什么意思| 经常玩手机有什么危害| 呼吸困难胸闷气短挂什么科| 小孩吐奶是什么原因| 吃什么开胃增加食欲| 橘子什么季节成熟| 装清高是什么意思| 整形什么医院好| 黄金是什么| 11月25日什么星座| 荷花代表什么象征意义| 天秤座的幸运色是什么| 山茶花什么颜色| 拉烂屎是什么原因| 什么情况啊这是| 百度Jump to content

一天吃几个鸡蛋比较好?

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Humpback whale breaching
百度 国内燃料电池汽车市场尚处于初级阶段,以已获得双资质认证的五家电动汽车龙头企业为例,只有长江汽车拥有完整的燃料电池技术储备和车型储备。

Cetacean surfacing behaviour is a grouping of movement types that cetaceans make at the water's surface in addition to breathing. Cetaceans have developed and use surface behaviours for many functions such as display, feeding and communication. All regularly observed members of the infraorder Cetacea, including whales, dolphins and porpoises, show a range of surfacing behaviours.

Cetacea is usually split into two suborders, Odontoceti and Mysticeti, based on the presence of teeth or baleen plates in adults respectively. However, when considering behaviour, Cetacea can be split into whales (cetaceans more than 10 m long such as sperm and most baleen whales) and dolphins and porpoises (all Odontocetes less than 10 m long including orca[1]) as many behaviours are correlated with size.

Although some behaviours such as spyhopping, logging and lobtailing occur in both groups, others such as bow riding or peduncle throws are exclusive to one or the other. It is these energetic behaviours that humans observe most frequently, which has resulted in a large amount of scientific literature on the subject and a popular tourism industry.

Travelling surface behaviour

[edit]

Breaching and lunging

[edit]
Humpback whale breach sequence

A breach or a lunge is a leap out of the water, also known as cresting. The distinction between the two is fairly arbitrary: cetacean researcher Hal Whitehead defines a breach as any leap in which at least 40% of the animal's body clears the water, and a lunge as a leap with less than 40% clearance.[2] Qualitatively, a breach is a genuine jump with an intent to clear the water, whereas a lunge is the result of a fast upward-sloping swim that has caused the whale to clear the surface of the water unintentionally. This latter "lunging" behaviour is often a result of feeding in rorquals.[3] The right, humpback, and sperm whales are the most widely observed jumpers. However other baleen whales such as fin, blue, minke, gray and sei whales also breach. Oceanic dolphins, including the orca, are very common breachers and are in fact capable of lifting themselves completely out of the water very easily, although there is little distinction between this and porpoising. Some non-cetacean marine creatures also exhibit breaching behavior, such as several shark species and rays of the genera Manta and Mobula.[4]

Two techniques are used by cetaceans in order to breach. The first method, most common in sperm and humpback whales, is conducted by swimming vertically upwards from depth, and heading straight out of the water.[5] The other more common method is to travel close to the surface and parallel to it, and then jerk upwards at full speed with as few as 3 tail strokes to perform a breach.[5][6] In all breaches the cetacean clears the water with the majority of its body at an acute angle, such as an average of 30° to the horizontal as recorded in sperm whales.[7] The whale then turns to land on its back or side, and less frequently may not turn but "belly flop" instead. In order to achieve 90% clearance, a humpback needs to leave the water at a speed of eight metres per second or 29 kilometres per hour (18 mph). For a 36 metric tons (40 short tons) animal, this results in a momentum of 288 thousand newton seconds. Despite its energetic cost, breaching is often carried out in series. The longest recorded sustained series was by a humpback near the West Indies totaling 130 leaps in less than 90 minutes.[8] Repeated breaches tire the animal, so less of the body clears the water each time.[9]

Ultimately, the reasons for breaching are unknown; however, there is evidence to support a range of hypotheses. Whales are more likely to breach when they are in groups, suggesting that it is a non-verbal signal to other group members during social behaviour. Scientists have called this theory "honest signalling". The immense cloud of bubbles and underwater disturbance following a breach cannot be faked; neighbours then know a breach has taken place. A single breach costs a whale only about 0.075% of its total daily energy intake, but a long series of breaches may add up to a significant energy expenditure.[9] A breach is therefore a sign that the animal is physically fit enough to afford energy for this acrobatic display, hence it could be used for ascertaining dominance, courting or warning of danger.[5] It is also possible that the loud "smack" upon re-entering is useful for stunning or scaring prey, similar to lobtailing. As breaching is often seen in rough seas it is possible that a breach allows the whale to breathe in air that is not close to the surface and full of spray, or that they use breaching to communicate when the noise of the ocean would mask acoustic signals.[10] Another widely accepted possible reason is to dislodge parasites from the skin.[10] The behaviour may also be more simply a form of play.[10]

Porpoising

[edit]

Porpoising, also known as running,[11] is a high speed surface behaviour of small cetaceans where long jumps are alternated with swimming close to the surface. Despite the name, porpoising behaviour is seen in dolphins and porpoises, as well as other marine species such as penguins[12] and pinnipeds.[13] When marine mammals are travelling at speed they are forced to stay close to the surface in order to maintain respiration for the energetic exercise. At leisurely cruising speeds below 4.6 m/s, dolphins swim below the water's surface and only briefly expose their blowholes along with up to one third of their body at any one time.[11] This results in little splashing as they have a very streamlined shape.[13] Porpoising occurs mainly when dolphins and porpoises are swimming at speeds greater than 4.6 m/s.[11] Here, jump length is roughly equal to distance traveled when the cetaceans are submerged.[11] This exposes the blowhole for longer which is needed to get enough oxygen to maintain metabolism and therefore high speeds over long periods of time. Studies have also shown that leaping is more energetically efficient than swimming above a certain threshold speed.[11] This is due to the reduction in friction when travelling in air compared to water which saves more energy than is needed to produce the leap.[13] These benefits also outweigh the energy wasted due to the large amount of splashing often seen when groups are porpoising.[11] Porpoising is therefore a result of high speed swimming which cetaceans use for important pursuit and escape activities. For example, dolphins may be seen porpoising away from their main predator, sharks[14] or the direction of incoming boats to avoid collision.[15]

Although porpoising is a useful product of rapid swimming, much variation seen in the behaviour cannot be explained by this cause alone; it has likely evolved to provide other functions. For example, the rotation during porpoising by the spinner dolphin leads to much splashing and is more common at slower speeds[11] so cannot be attributed to an energy saving mechanism. It is therefore more likely to be a form of play or communication within or between pods.[11] Another reason might be to remove barnacles or remoras that, when attached, increase drag during swimming.[16] When spinner dolphins impact the water the combination of centrifugal and vertical force upon these ectoparasites can be up to 700 times their own weight and so efficiently remove them.[16] Other theories suggest that cetaceans may porpoise in order to observe distant objects such as food by looking for visual cues, such as birds dive-bombing a bait ball.[17] Research into the additional functions of porpoising has so far been focussed on the more acrobatic species, but it is likely that other cetaceans also use it for these, and perhaps unknown, reasons too.

Wave or bow-riding and following vessels

[edit]

The term wave-riding is most commonly used to describe the surface activity of cetaceans that approach boats and jump repeatedly in the waves produced by the boats. This includes bow-riding, where cetaceans are in the pressure wave in front of the boat, and wake-riding, where they are off the stern in the wake.[18] Cetaceans swim using fluke propulsion when experiencing wave energy below the threshold needed for riding, such as when boats travel at speeds slower than 3 m/s[19] or when they are outside of the peak wave energy zone. However, at higher speeds dolphins and porpoises will seek out the pressure wave and its maximum energy zone in order to ride the wave by holding their flukes in a fixed plane, with only minor adjustments for repositioning.[19] Wave-riding reduces the energetic cost of swimming to the dolphin, even when compared to slower swimming speeds.[19] For example, heart rate, metabolic rate and transport cost was reduced by up to 70% during wave-riding compared to swimming at speeds 1 m/s slower in bottlenose dolphin.[19] Wave-riding behaviour can be performed by dolphins from minutes up to several hours,[19] and therefore is a useful energy-saving mechanism for swimming at higher speeds.

Wave-riding is most common in small Odontocetes. It has also been observed in larger cetaceans such as false killer whales and orca,[20][21] although most larger Odontocetes do not seek out any form of interaction with boats. Bow-riding is the most common form of interactive behaviour with boats across a variety of smaller Odontocete species, such as dolphins in the genera Stenella and Delphinus.[22] The type of interaction can often depend on the behavioral state of the group as well as species. For example, spotted dolphins are more likely to interact when travelling or milling but less likely when they are socialising or surface feeding.[22] Interactive behavior may also depend on group composition, as both orca and bottlenose dolphins have been recorded to interact mostly when a calf was in the group.[22][23] This indicates that groups with calves may approach boats in order to teach the young how to interact safely to avoid collision. Another result of cetaceans traveling in pods is an increase in competition for the optimal wave energy and so maximum energy saving position. Position of individuals may reflect the dominance hierarchy of the pod and therefore could be used to ascertain dominance.[21] Several rorquals, such as minke,[24] sei,[25] bryde's,[26] humpback,[27] and gray[28] are also known to display actions in similar manners.

[edit]

Stationary surface behaviour

[edit]

Spyhopping

[edit]

When spyhopping, the whale rises and holds a vertical position partially out of the water, often exposing its entire rostrum and head. It is visually akin to a human treading water. Spyhopping is controlled and slow, and can last for minutes at a time if the whale is sufficiently inquisitive about whatever it is viewing. Generally, the whale does not appear to swim by fluke propulsion to maintain its "elevated" position while spyhopping, instead relying on exceptional buoyancy control and positioning with pectoral fins. Typically the whale's eyes will be slightly above or below the surface of the water, enabling it to see whatever is nearby on the surface.[29] Different species of sharks, including the great white shark and oceanic whitetip shark, have also been known to spyhop.[30][31]

Spyhopping often occurs during a "mugging" situation, where the focus of a whale's attention is on a boat, such as whale-watching tours, which they sometimes approach and interact with.[32] On the other hand, spyhopping among orcas is thought to aid predation, as they are often seen around ice floes attempting to view prey species such as seals that are resting on the floes.[33] When prey is detected the individual will conduct a series of spy-hops from different locations around it, then vocalise to the group members to do the same to possibly prepare for an attack.[33] In this instance a spyhop may be more useful than a breach, because the view is held steady for a longer period of time. Often when cetaceans breach, their eyes do not clear the water, which suggests it might not be used for looking but instead for hearing. For example, gray whales will often spy-hop in order to hear better when they are near the line where waves begin to break in the ocean as this marks out their migration route.[29]

Lobtailing and slapping

[edit]

Lobtailing is the act of a whale or dolphin lifting its flukes out of the water and then bringing them down onto the surface of the water hard and fast in order to make a loud slap. Large whales tend to lobtail by positioning themselves vertically downwards into the water and then slapping the surface by bending the tail stock. Dolphins, however, tend to remain horizontal, either on their belly or their back, and make the slap via a jerky whole body movement. All species are likely to slap several times in a single session. Like breaching, lobtailing is common amongst active cetacean species such as sperm, humpback, right and gray whales. It is less common, but still occasionally occurs, amongst the other large whales. Porpoises and river dolphins rarely lobtail, but it is a very common phenomenon amongst oceanic dolphins. Lobtailing is more common within species that have a complex social order than those where animals are more likely to be solitary. Lobtailing often occurs in conjunction with other aerial behaviour such as breaching. Species with large flippers may also slap them against the water for a similar effect, known as pectoral slapping.[citation needed]

The sound of a lobtail can be heard underwater several hundred metres from the site of a slap. This has led to speculation amongst scientists that lobtailing is, like breaching, a form of non-vocal communication. However, studies of bowhead whales have shown that the noise of a lobtail travels much less well than that of a vocal call or a breach. Thus the lobtail is probably important visually as well as acoustically, and may be a sign of aggression. Some suggest that lobtailing in humpback whales is a means of foraging. The hypothesis is that the loud noise causes fish to become frightened, thus tightening their school together, making it easier for the humpback to feed on them.[34] In this instance, lobtail feeding behaviour appeared to progressively spread throughout the population, as it increased from 0 to 50% of the population using it over the 9-year study.[34] As no individual under 2 years old nor any mothers were observed to use lobtail feeding it suggests that it is taught in foraging groups. The spread of lobtail feeding amongst humpback whales indicates its success as a novel foraging method.[34]

[edit]

Peduncle throw

[edit]

A peduncle throw, also known as peduncling, is a surfacing behaviour unique to humpback whales. During this the humpback converts its forward momentum into a crack-the-whip rotation, pivoting with its pectorals as it drives its head downward and thrusts its entire fluke and peduncle (the muscular rear portion of the torso) out of the water and sideways, before crashing into the water with terrific force. Peduncling takes place among the focal animals (female, escort, challenging male) in a competitive group, apparently as an aggressive gesture. Possibilities include escorts fending off a particular challenging male, females who seem agitated with an escort, or an individual not comfortable with a watching boat's presence. Occasionally, one whale performs a series of dozens of peduncle throws, directed at the same target each time.[35]

Pectoral slapping

[edit]

Pectoral slapping, informally known as pec-slapping, is when a cetacean turns on its side, exposes one or both pectoral fins into the air, and then slaps them against the surface of the water. It is a form of non-vocal communication[36] commonly observed in a variety of whale and dolphin species as well as seals. The motion is slow and controlled, and the behaviour can occur repeatedly by one individual over a few minutes.[37] The humpback whale's pectoral fin is the largest appendage of any mammal and humpbacks are known for their extremely acrobatic behaviour. Pec-slapping varies between groups of different social structure, such as not occurring in lone males but being common in mother calf pairs and also when they are accompanied by an escort.[37] The reasons for pec-slapping therefore can vary depending on age and sex of individual humpback whales. During the breeding season adult males pec-slap before they disassociate with a group of males that are vying for a female, whereas adult females pec-slap to attract potential mates and indicate that she is sexually receptive.[38] Its function between mother calf pairs is less well known but is likely to be a form of play and communication that is taught to the calf by the mother for use when it is sexually mature.[38] Pectoral slapping has also been observed in the right whale, but due to its smaller size, the sound produced will be quieter[39] and therefore used for communication over smaller distances unlike the humpback. Exposure of the pectoral fin and consequent slapping has also been infrequently observed in blue whales, where it is most often a by-product of lunge feeding followed by rolling on to its side.

Logging

[edit]

Logging is a behaviour that whales exhibit when at rest and appear like "logs" at the surface.[40] It is defined as lying without forward movement at the surface of the water with the dorsal fin or parts of the back are exposed.[41] Whales often rest for periods of time under the surface in order to sleep in mainly horizontal positions, although sperm whales also rest vertically.[42] However, as they consciously need to breathe at the surface, they can rest only one-half of their brain at a time, known as unihemispheric slow-wave sleep. This sleep pattern has been identified in all five cetacean species that have been tested for it thus far.[43] Cetaceans intermittently come to the surface in order to breathe during these sleep periods and exhibit logging behaviour. Logging can occur interchangeably with surface resting behaviour when cetaceans are travelling slowly, which is particularly common in mother-calf pairs,[44] as the young tire quickly during swimming. Logging is common, particularly in right whales, sperm whales, pilot whales and humpback whales. Another behaviour that may be mistaken for logging is milling, where a group of cetaceans at the surface have little or no directional movement[45] but instead socialise with each other. This behaviour is particularly common in large groups of pilot whales.[45]

Dive times

[edit]

Time intervals between surfacing can vary depending on the species, surfacing style or the purpose of the dive; some species have been known to dive for up to 85 minutes at a time when hunting,[46] and dives in excess of three hours have been observed in Cuvier's beaked whale under extreme circumstances.[47]

Human interaction

[edit]

Whale watching is carried out on every continent, with an estimated 13 million people participating in 2008.[48] This, when combined with the sustained increase in boat vessel traffic, has likely affected the surface activity of cetaceans. When boats and other whale watching vessels approach, most cetaceans will either avoid or seek interactions. The occasions where no effect is seen is predominantly when the cetaceans are travelling or feeding, but not when they are showing surface activity.[49] In the case of avoidance, the animals may dive rather than staying submerged near the surface or move horizontally away from the vessels.[50] For example, when sperm whales are approached by boats they surface less, shorten the intervals between breathes and do not show their fluke before diving as often.[49] Cetaceans may also reduce their acrobatic surfacing behaviours, such as when humpback whale groups without calves are approached by vessels to within 300 m.[51] Avoidance behaviour is typical of whales, but interactions are more common in whale groups that contain calves[50] and also in the smaller odontocetes. For example, studies on killer whales in North America have shown that the focal animals increased their tail-slapping behaviour when approached by boats within 100 m, and that 70% of surface active behaviours (SABs) in these orca were seen when a boat was within 225 m.[52] Similarly, dusky dolphins also jump, change direction and form tighter groups more when boats are present, particularly when they do not adhere to the regulations about approach.[53] As an increase in SABs is beneficial to the whale watching tours' participants, the tours may be encouraged to approach cetaceans closer than recommended by guidelines. There is a lack of understanding about the long-term effects of whale-watching on the behaviour of cetaceans, but it is theorised that it may cause avoidance of popular sites,[51] or a decrease in the energy budget for individuals involved.[50]

See also

[edit]


References

[edit]
  1. ^ [1][dead link], FAO Marine Mammals of the World, Suborder Odontoceti.
  2. ^ Whitehead Lab, Publications List, retrieved 2025-08-06
  3. ^ Ware, C.; Friedlaender, A. S.; Nowacek, D. P. (2011), "Shallow and deep lunge feeding of humpback whales in fjords of the West Antarctic Peninsula", Marine Mammal Science, 27 (3): 587–605, Bibcode:2011MMamS..27..587W, doi:10.1111/j.1748-7692.2010.00427.x
  4. ^ Paul and Michael Albert (26 June 2005). "The Flying Mobulas of the Sea of Cortez". Archived from the original on 4 May 2013.
  5. ^ a b c The Dodo, This is how whales break through the surface of the water, archived from the original on 2025-08-06, retrieved 2025-08-06
  6. ^ Breaching, Conscious Breath Adventures, retrieved 2025-08-06
  7. ^ Hal Whitehead (2003), Sperm whales: social evolution in the ocean, University of Chicago Press, p. 179, ISBN 978-0-226-89517-8
  8. ^ "Whale Behaviour". Animal Corner. 26 February 2015.
  9. ^ a b Whitehead, Hal (2003), Sperm Whales: Social Evolution in the Ocean, Chicago: University of Chicago Press, p. 181, ISBN 978-0-226-89517-8
  10. ^ a b c USCB ScienceLine, Why do whales and other sea mammals breach?
  11. ^ a b c d e f g h Au, D.; Weihs, D. (1980), "At high speeds dolphins save energy by leaping.", Nature, 284 (5756): 548–550, Bibcode:1980Natur.284..548A, doi:10.1038/284548a0, S2CID 4334691
  12. ^ Hui, C. A. (1987), "The porpoising of penguins an energy-conserving behaviour for respiratory ventilation?", Canadian Journal of Zoology, 65: 209–211, doi:10.1139/z87-031
  13. ^ a b c Weihs, D. (2002), "Dynamics of dolphin porpoising revisited", Integrative and Comparative Biology, 42 (5): 1071–1078, doi:10.1093/icb/42.5.1071, PMID 21680390
  14. ^ Heithaus, M. R. (2001), "Shark attacks on bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus) in Shark Bay, Western Australia: attack rate, bite scar frequencies, and attack seasonality", Marine Mammal Science, 17 (3): 526–539, Bibcode:2001MMamS..17..526H, doi:10.1111/j.1748-7692.2001.tb01002.x
  15. ^ Lusseau, D. (2003), "Effects of tour boats on the behavior of bottlenose dolphins: using Markov chains to model anthropogenic impacts", Conservation Biology, 17 (6): 1785–1793, Bibcode:2003ConBi..17.1785L, doi:10.1111/j.1523-1739.2003.00054.x, S2CID 83589031
  16. ^ a b Weihs, D.; Fish, F. E.; Nicastro, A. J. (2007), "Mechanics of remora removal by dolphin spinning", Marine Mammal Science, 23 (3): 707–714, Bibcode:2007MMamS..23..707W, doi:10.1111/j.1748-7692.2007.00131.x
  17. ^ [2], "Why do dolphins jump out of the water?", Dolphin World FAQ.
  18. ^ Miller, L. J.; Solangi, M.; Kuczaj, S. A. (2010), "Seasonal and Diurnal Patterns of Behavior Exhibited by Atlantic Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) in the Mississippi Sound", Ethology, 116 (12): 1127–1137, Bibcode:2010Ethol.116.1127M, doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.2010.01824.x
  19. ^ a b c d e Williams, T. M.; Friedl, W. A.; Fong, M. L.; Yamada, R. M.; Sedivy, P.; Huan, J. E. (1992), "Travel at low energetic cost by swimming and wave-riding bottlenose dolphins", Nature, 355 (6363): 821–823, Bibcode:1992Natur.355..821W, doi:10.1038/355821a0, PMID 1538760, S2CID 4281153
  20. ^ Rafael Avitabile (10 September 2018). "Fishing charter crew captures orca pod playing in boat's wake". NBC News - San Diego. Retrieved 5 November 2020.
  21. ^ a b Dahlheim, M. E. (1980), "Killer whales observed bowriding", The Murrelet, 61 (2): 78–80, JSTOR 3535038
  22. ^ a b c Ritter, F. (2003), "Interactions of cetaceans with whale watching boats–implications for the management of whale watching tourism" (PDF), MEER EV, Berlin, Germany, archived from the original (PDF) on 2025-08-06
  23. ^ Hawkins, E.; Gartside, D. F. (2009), "Interactive behaviours of bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops aduncus) during encounters with vessels", Aquatic Mammals, 35 (2): 259–268, doi:10.1578/AM.35.2.2009.259
  24. ^ "La baleine s'amuse by Gilles Macé". PBase. Retrieved 24 September 2018.
  25. ^ 橘田佳音利 (7 March 2014). "クジラの大群". Archived from the original on 2025-08-06. Retrieved 24 September 2018 – via YouTube.
  26. ^ "Boná Island and Panama Canal, Panama". Expeditions.com. January 14, 2016.
  27. ^ "Bowriding Humpback Whales – Log Transcript". PBS. December 30, 2002.
  28. ^ "2 gray whales catch waves near Mission Beach". FOX5 San Diego. February 3, 2016.
  29. ^ a b Gray Whale Spyhop, National Geographic, archived from the original on 2025-08-06, retrieved 2025-08-06
  30. ^ Baldwin, Marc. "Elasmobranch Behaviour". Wildlife Online. Retrieved August 7, 2020.
  31. ^ Keartes, Sarah (October 5, 2016). "What is the shark-steps-on-Lego shark actually doing?". Earth Touch News. Retrieved August 7, 2020.
  32. ^ Ng, S. L.; Leung, S. (2003), "Behavioral response of Indo-Pacific humpback dolphin (Sousa chinensis) to vessel traffic", Marine Environmental Research, 56 (5): 555–567, Bibcode:2003MarER..56..555N, doi:10.1016/S0141-1136(03)00041-2, PMID 12927738
  33. ^ a b Pitman, R. L.; Durban, J. W. (2012), "Cooperative hunting behavior, prey selectivity and prey handling by pack ice killer whales (Orcinus orca), type B, in Antarctic Peninsula waters", Marine Mammal Science, 28 (1): 16–36, Bibcode:2012MMamS..28...16P, doi:10.1111/j.1748-7692.2010.00453.x
  34. ^ a b c Weinrich, Mason T.; Schilling, Mark R.; Belt, Cynthia R. (December 1992), "Evidence for acquisition of a novel feeding behaviour: lobtail feeding in humpback whales, Megaptera novaeangliae", Animal Behaviour, 44 (6): 1059–1072, doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80318-5, S2CID 53270820
  35. ^ Dr Stavros Kaniklides, The effects of volcanic tsunamis on marine mammals, retrieved 2025-08-06
  36. ^ Kavanagh, A. S., Owen, K., Williamson, M. J., Noad, M. J., Goldizen, A. W., Kniest, E., Cato, D. H., Dunlop, R. A. (January 2017). "Evidence for the functions of surface-active behaviors in humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae)". Marine Mammal Science. 33 (1): 313–334. Bibcode:2017MMamS..33..313K. doi:10.1111/mms.12374 – via ResearchGate.{{cite journal}}: CS1 maint: multiple names: authors list (link)
  37. ^ a b Dunlop, R. A.; Cato, D. H.; Noad, M. J. (2008), "Non-song acoustic communication in migrating humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae).", Marine Mammal Science, 24 (3): 613–629, Bibcode:2008MMamS..24..613D, doi:10.1111/j.1748-7692.2008.00208.x
  38. ^ a b Deakos, M. H. (2012), "Humpback whale (Megaptera novaeangliae) communication: The context and potential functions of pec-slapping behavior on the Hawaiian wintering grounds.", Doctoral Dissertation, University of Hawaii at Manoa (Thesis), hdl:10125/7066
  39. ^ "North Atlantic right whale". Arkive. Archived from the original on 2025-08-06. Retrieved 2025-08-06.
  40. ^ Harris, Tom (13 July 2001), "How Whales Work", Howstuffworks, retrieved 2025-08-06
  41. ^ "Whales" (PDF), Marine Discovery Centre, Henley Beach, S. A., archived from the original (PDF) on 28 August 2006, retrieved 17 November 2024
  42. ^ Miller, P. J. O.; Aoki, K.; Rendell, L. E.; Amano, M. (2008), "Stereotypical resting behaviour of the sperm whale.", Current Biology, 18 (1): R21 – R23, Bibcode:2008CBio...18..R21M, doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.11.003, PMID 18177706, S2CID 10587736
  43. ^ Lyamin, O. I.; Mukhametov, L. M.; Seigel, J. M.; Nazarenko, E. A.; Polyakova, I. G.; Shpak, O. V. (2002), "Unihemispheric slow wave sleep and the state of the eyes in a white whales", Behavioural Brain Research, 129 (1–2): 125–129, doi:10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00346-1, PMC 8788623, PMID 11809503, S2CID 27009685
  44. ^ "Why do the Humpback Whales visit Hervey Bay?", The Oceania Project, retrieved 2025-08-06
  45. ^ a b Weilgart, L. S.; Whitehead, H. (1990), "Vocalizations of the North Atlantic pilot whale (Globicephala melas) as related to behavioral contexts", Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 26 (6): 399–402, Bibcode:1990BEcoS..26..399W, doi:10.1007/BF00170896, S2CID 34187605
  46. ^ "The Deepest Divers", Oceanus Magazine, Woods Hole Oceanographic Institute.
  47. ^ Quick, Nicola J.; Cioffi, William R.; Shearer, Jeanne M.; Fahlman, Andreas; Read, Andrew J. (15 September 2020). "Extreme diving in mammals: first estimates of behavioural aerobic dive limits in Cuvier's beaked whales". The Journal of Experimental Biology. 223 (18): jeb222109. doi:10.1242/jeb.222109. PMID 32967976. S2CID 221886321.
  48. ^ O'Connor, S.; Campbell, R.; Cortez, H.; Knowles, T. (2009), "Whale Watching Worldwide: tourism numbers, expenditures and expanding economic benefits: a special report from the International Fund for Animal Welfare, Yarmouth MA, USA.", Economists at Large, archived from the original on August 1, 2012
  49. ^ a b Gordon, J.; Leaper, R.; Hartley, F. G.; Chappell, O. (1992), "Effects of whale-watching vessels on the surface and underwater acoustic behaviour of sperm whales off Kaikoura, New Zealand." (PDF), Wellington, N.Z.: Head Office, Department of Conservation, retrieved 2025-08-06
  50. ^ a b c Stamation, K. A.; Croft, D. B.; Shaughnessy, P. D.; Waples, K. A.; Briggs, S. V. (2010), "Behavioral responses of humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae) to whale-watching vessels on the southeastern coast of Australia", Marine Mammal Science, 26 (1): 98–122, Bibcode:2010MMamS..26...98S, doi:10.1111/j.1748-7692.2009.00320.x
  51. ^ a b Corkeron, P. J. (1995), "Humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae) in Hervey Bay, Queensland: behaviour and responses to whale-watching vessels", Canadian Journal of Zoology, 73 (7): 1290–1299, doi:10.1139/z95-153
  52. ^ Noren, D. P.; Johnson, A. H.; Rehder, D.; Larson, A. (2009), "Close approaches by vessels elicit surface active behaviors by southern resident killer whales.", Endangered Species Research, 8 (3): 179–192, doi:10.3354/esr00205
  53. ^ Barr, K.; Slooten, E. (1999), "Effects of tourism on dusky dolphins at Kaikoura." (PDF), Wellington: Department of Conservation, retrieved 2025-08-06

Further reading

[edit]
[edit]
发声是什么意思 怀孕检查挂什么科 蟹爪兰什么时候开花 2018年是什么命 爱恨就在一瞬间是什么歌
脸部肌肉跳动是什么原因 蛋白质是什么食物 耳朵发烧是什么原因 滑板鞋是什么鞋 程门立雪是什么生肖
早泄挂什么科 三月六日是什么星座 看病人带什么水果 拉肚子吃什么 夏天喝绿茶有什么好处
甘露醇是什么药 山楂泡水喝有什么好处 孩子说话晚是什么原因 甘油三酯高有什么危害 阿昔洛韦乳膏治什么病
疤痕增生是什么引起的hcv8jop6ns9r.cn 硫酸铜什么颜色hcv9jop6ns4r.cn 脊柱侧弯有什么症状hcv8jop0ns6r.cn a和b生的孩子是什么血型hcv9jop2ns9r.cn 桃字五行属什么zhongyiyatai.com
京东plus是什么意思hcv8jop8ns7r.cn 二次元文化是什么意思hcv7jop9ns3r.cn 小儿电解质补给液有什么作用xinjiangjialails.com 母亲节在什么时候0297y7.com 治类风湿用什么方法好hcv8jop3ns2r.cn
hkc是什么牌子hcv7jop4ns8r.cn 甲状腺一度肿大是什么意思hcv8jop9ns9r.cn 五点多是什么时辰hcv7jop9ns3r.cn 铁杵是什么hcv8jop3ns2r.cn 湿气重是什么原因引起的hcv8jop1ns8r.cn
电磁炉用什么锅最好gangsutong.com 白细胞酯酶弱阳性什么意思hcv8jop2ns9r.cn 毕业穿的衣服叫什么cl108k.com 淋巴结挂什么科hcv8jop8ns5r.cn 肺部感染是什么症状hcv8jop2ns9r.cn
百度